Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Review Article
  • Published:

The right time to learn: mechanisms and optimization of spaced learning

Key Points

  • Spaced learning or training is robustly superior to massed training for many forms of human learning and for virtually every animal model system that has been examined. The phenomenon has been a major focus of learning theorists for the past 50 years.

  • Recent cellular and molecular data have begun to elucidate mechanisms for the efficacy of spaced training. Dynamics of signalling cascades, dendritic spine remodelling and transcription cooperate to sustain this efficacy.

  • Some of these data appear to align with a traditional learning theory, deficient-processing theory.

  • Computational models of signalling cascades can be used to predict irregularly spaced protocols to enhance learning or rescue learning deficits.

  • Models further suggest ways in which combining pharmacotherapy with optimized spaced protocols can further enhance learning. This strategy may have promise for improving learning in some human cognitive disorders.

Abstract

For many types of learning, spaced training, which involves repeated long inter-trial intervals, leads to more robust memory formation than does massed training, which involves short or no intervals. Several cognitive theories have been proposed to explain this superiority, but only recently have data begun to delineate the underlying cellular and molecular mechanisms of spaced training, and we review these theories and data here. Computational models of the implicated signalling cascades have predicted that spaced training with irregular inter-trial intervals can enhance learning. This strategy of using models to predict optimal spaced training protocols, combined with pharmacotherapy, suggests novel ways to rescue impaired synaptic plasticity and learning.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Figure 1: Early conceptual model of how learning trace dynamics generate an optimal interval.
Figure 2: Model and hypotheses describing synaptic strengthening during spaced learning.
Figure 3: Different mechanisms may underlie enhancement of learning by spaced intervals of widely varying lengths.
Figure 4: Dynamics of a model that has successfully predicted greater efficacy for a learning protocol with irregularly spaced intervals.
Figure 5: A model predicts that a pair of drugs can act synergistically to enhance LTP.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Ebbinghaus, H. Memory; a Contribution to Experimental Psychology Ch. 2 (Teachers College, Columbia University, 1913).

    Google Scholar 

  2. Cepeda, N. J., Pashler, H., Vul, E., Wixted, J. T. & Rohrer, D. Distributed practice in verbal recall tasks: a review and quantitative synthesis. Psychol. Bull. 132, 354–380 (2006). This extensive review and meta-analysis delineates the comprehensive body of knowledge describing human spaced and massed verbal learning, as well as theories posited to explain the superiority of spaced training over massed training in terms of inducing long-term memory formation.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Godbole, N. R., Delaney, P. F. & Verkoeijen, P. P. The spacing effect in immediate and delayed free recall. Memory 22, 462–469 (2014).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Raman, M. et al. Teaching in small portions dispersed over time enhances long-term knowledge retention. Med. Teach. 32, 250–255 (2010).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Donovan, J. J. & Radosevich, D. J. A meta-analytic review of the distribution of practice effect. J. Appl. Psychol. 84, 795–805 (1999).

    Google Scholar 

  6. Shea, C. H., Lai, Q., Black, C. & Park, J. H. Spacing practice sessions across days benefits the learning of motor skills. Hum. Mov. Sci. 19, 737–760 (2000).

    Google Scholar 

  7. Gluckman, M., Vlach, H. A. & Sandhofer, C. M. Spacing simultaneously promotes multiple forms of learning in children's science curriculum. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 28, 266–273 (2014).

    Google Scholar 

  8. McDaniel, M. A. & Callender, A. A. in Learning and Memory: A Comprehensive Reference Vol. 2 (ed. Roediger, H. L.) 819–844 (Academic Press, 2008).

    Google Scholar 

  9. Sobel, H. S., Cepeda, N. J. & Kapler, I. V. Spacing effects in real-world classroom vocabulary learning. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 25, 763–767 (2011).

    Google Scholar 

  10. Vlach, H. A. The spacing effect in children's generalization of knowledge: allowing children time to forget promotes their ability to learn. Child Dev. Persp. 8, 163–168 (2014). This study found that, in children's education, spacing of learning sessions promoted not only better fact retention but also generalization of currently learned items to bind with and extend previously established conceptual categories.

    Google Scholar 

  11. Philips, G. T., Kopec, A. M. & Carew, T. J. Pattern and predictability in memory formation: from molecular mechanisms to clinical relevance. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 105, 117–124 (2013).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Philips, G. T., Ye, X., Kopec, A. M. & Carew, T. J. MAPK establishes a molecular context that defines effective training patterns for long-term memory formation. J. Neurosci. 33, 7565–7573 (2013). This work characterized the dynamics of MAPK and its activation of C/EBP, a transcription factor that is important for long-term memory formation, identifying the response of MAPK within a specific time window as essential for generating an optimal inter-trial interval.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Wainwright, M. L., Zhang, H., Byrne, J. H. & Cleary, L. J. Localized neuronal outgrowth induced by long-term sensitization training in Aplysia. J. Neurosci. 22, 4132–4141 (2002).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Mauelshagen, J., Sherff, C. M. & Carew, T. J. Differential induction of long-term synaptic facilitation by spaced and massed applications of serotonin at sensory neuron synapses of Aplysia californica. Learn. Mem. 5, 246–256 (1998).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Beck, C. D., Schroeder, B. & Davis, R. L. Learning performance of normal and mutant Drosophila after repeated conditioning trials with discrete stimuli. J. Neurosci. 20, 2944–2953 (2000).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Pagani, M. R., Oishi, K., Gelb, B. D. & Zhong, Y. The phosphatase SHP2 regulates the spacing effect for long-term memory induction. Cell 139, 186–198 (2009).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Menzel, R., Manz, G., Menzel, R. & Greggers, U. Massed and spaced learning in honeybees: the role of CS, US, the intertrial interval, and the test interval. Learn. Mem. 8, 198–208 (2001).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Anderson, M. J., Jablonski, S. A. & Klimas, D. B. Spaced initial stimulus familiarization enhances novelty preference in Long-Evans rats. Behav. Processes 78, 481–486 (2008).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Bello-Medina, P. C., Sanchez-Carrasco, L., Gonzalez-Ornelas, N. R., Jeffery, K. J. & Ramirez-Amaya, V. Differential effects of spaced versus massed training in long-term object-identity and object-location recognition memory. Behav. Brain Res. 250, 102–113 (2013).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Medin, D. L. The comparative study of memory. J. Hum. Evol. 3, 455–463 (1974).

    Google Scholar 

  21. Robbins, O. & Bush, C. T. Memory in great apes. J. Exp. Psychol. 97, 344–348 (1973).

    Google Scholar 

  22. Urcelay, G. P., Wheeler, D. S. & Miller, R. R. Spacing extinction trials alleviates renewal and spontaneous recovery. Learn. Behav. 37, 60–73 (2009).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Benjamin, A. S. & Tullis, J. What makes distributed practice effective? Cogn. Psychol. 61, 228–247 (2010).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Madigan, S. A. Intraserial repetition and coding processes in free recall. J. Verb. Learn. Verb. Behav. 8, 828–835 (1969).

    Google Scholar 

  25. Melton, A. W. The situation with respect to the spacing of repetitions and memory. J. Verb. Learn. Verb. Behav. 9, 596–606 (1970).

    Google Scholar 

  26. Braun, K. & Rubin, D. C. The spacing effect depends on an encoding deficit, retrieval, and time in working memory: evidence from once-presented words. Memory 6, 37–65 (1998).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Greene, R. L. Spacing effects in memory: evidence for a two-process account. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 15, 371–377 (1989).

    Google Scholar 

  28. Hintzman, D. L., Summers, J. J. & Block, R. A. Spacing judgments as an index of study-phase retrieval. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Learn. 1, 31–40 (1975).

    Google Scholar 

  29. Tzeng, O. J. & Cotton, B. A study-phase retrieval model of temporal coding. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Learn. 6, 705–716 (1980).

    Google Scholar 

  30. Siegel, L. L. & Kahana, M. J. A retrieved context account of spacing and repetition effects in free recall. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 40, 755–764 (2014).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Hintzman, D. L. in Theories in Cognitive Psychology: The Loyola Symposium (ed. Solso, R. L.) 77–90 (Erlbaum, 1974).

    Google Scholar 

  32. Dempster, F. N. Spacing effects and their implications for theory and practice. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 1, 309–330 (1989).

    Google Scholar 

  33. Toppino, T. C. The spacing effect in young children's free recall: support for automatic-process explanations. Mem. Cognit. 19, 159–167 (1991).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Hintzman, D. L. in The Psychology of Learning and Motivation Vol. 10 (ed. Bower, G. H.) 47–91 (Academic Press, 1976).

    Google Scholar 

  35. Rundus, D. Analysis of rehearsal processes in free recall. J. Exp. Psychol. 89, 63–77 (1971).

    Google Scholar 

  36. Lechner, H. A., Squire, L. R. & Byrne, J. H. 100 years of consolidation — remembering Müller and Pilzecker. Learn. Mem. 6, 77–87 (1999).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Landauer, T. K. Reinforcement as consolidation. Psychol. Rev. 76, 82–96 (1969). This early paper posited and described elements of the dynamics of spaced learning, such as the saturation of memory formation by closely spaced trials and the failure of reinforcement for long trial spacing, and how these elements generate an optimal inter-trial interval.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Peterson, L. R. Short-term verbal memory and learning. Psychol. Rev. 73, 193–207 (1966).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Wickelgren, W. A. Trace resistance and the decay of long-term memory. J. Math. Psychol. 9, 418–455 (1972).

    Google Scholar 

  40. Cuddy, L. J. & Jacoby, L. J. When forgetting helps memory: an analysis of repetition effects. J. Verb. Learn. Verb. Behav. 21, 451–467 (1982).

    Google Scholar 

  41. Toppino, T. C. & Bloom, L. C. The spacing effect, free recall, and two-process theory: a closer look. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 28, 437–444 (2002).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Nabavi, S. et al. Engineering a memory with LTD and LTP. Nature 511, 348–352 (2014).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  43. Bosch, M. & Hayashi, Y. Structural plasticity of dendritic spines. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 22, 383–388 (2012).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Bell, M. E. et al. Dynamics of nascent and active zone ultrastructure as synapses enlarge during LTP in mature hippocampus. J. Comp. Neurol. 522, 3861–3864 (2014). This work, one of a group of related studies by the Harris laboratory, characterized in detail pre- and postsynaptic structural changes that are necessary for the consolidation of LTP.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Bourne, J. N. & Harris, K. M. Coordination of size and number of excitatory and inhibitory synapses results in a balanced structural plasticity along mature hippocampal CA1 dendrites during LTP. Hippocampus 21, 354–373 (2011).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. Cao, G. & Harris, K. M. Augmenting saturated LTP by broadly spaced episodes of theta-burst stimulation in hippocampal area CA1 of adult rats and mice. J. Neurophysiol. 112, 1916–1924 (2014).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  47. Kramár, E. A. et al. Synaptic evidence for the efficacy of spaced learning. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 109, 5121–5126 (2012). This study, together with reference 48, substantiates the key hypothesis that spaced stimuli first prime and then strengthen upon repetition successive groups of dendritic spines, creating a morphological correlate of the memory trace that spaced, but not massed, protocols effectively consolidate.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Lynch, G., Kramár, E. A., Babayan, A. H., Rumbaugh, G. & Gall, C. M. Differences between synaptic plasticity thresholds result in new timing rules for maximizing long-term potentiation. Neuropharmacology 64, 27–38 (2013).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Rex, C. S. et al. Different Rho GTPase-dependent signaling pathways initiate sequential steps in the consolidation of long-term potentiation. J. Cell Biol. 186, 85–97 (2009).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Nguyen, P. V., Abel, T. & Kandel, E. R. Requirement of a critical period of transcription for induction of a late phase of LTP. Science 265, 1104–1107 (1994).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Scharf, M. T. et al. Protein synthesis is required for the enhancement of long-term potentiation and long-term memory by spaced training. J. Neurophysiol. 87, 2770–2777 (2002).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Kandel, E. R. The molecular biology of memory storage: a dialogue between genes and synapses. Science 294, 1249–1254 (2001).

    Google Scholar 

  53. Silva, A. J., Kogan, J. H., Frankland, P. W. & Kida, S. CREB and memory. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 21, 127–148 (1998).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Liu, R. Y., Fioravante, D., Shah, S. & Byrne, J. H. cAMP response element-binding protein 1 feedback loop is necessary for consolidation of long-term synaptic facilitation in Aplysia. J. Neurosci. 28, 1970–1976 (2008).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Liu, R. Y., Cleary, L. J. & Byrne, J. H. The requirement for enhanced CREB1 expression in consolidation of long-term synaptic facilitation and long-term excitability in sensory neurons of Aplysia. J. Neurosci. 31, 6871–6879 (2011).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. Liu, R. Y., Shah, S., Cleary, L. J. & Byrne, J. H. Serotonin- and training-induced dynamic regulation of CREB2 in Aplysia. Learn. Mem. 18, 245–249 (2011).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  57. Frost, W. N., Castellucci, V., Hawkins, R. D. & Kandel, E. R. Monosynaptic connections made by the sensory neurons of the gill- and siphon-withdrawal reflex in Aplysia participate in the storage of long-term memory for sensitization. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 82, 8266–8269 (1985).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Pinsker, H. M., Hening, W. A., Carew, T. J. & Kandel, E. R. Long-term sensitization of a defensive withdrawal reflex in Aplysia. Science 182, 1039–1042 (1973).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Beckinschtein, P. et al. Persistence of long-term memory storage requires a late protein synthesis- and BDNF-dependent phase in the hippocampus. Neuron 53, 261–277 (2007).

    Google Scholar 

  60. Bambah-Mukku, D., Travaglia, A., Chen, D. Y., Pollonini, G. & Alberini, C. M. A positive autoregulatory BDNF feedback loop via C/EBPβ mediates hippocampal memory consolidation. J. Neurosci. 34, 12547–12559 (2014). This study characterizes the dynamics of a molecular positive feedback loop demonstrated to be important for the consolidation of long-term (24 hours or more) memory, as well as the late repression of Bdnf that terminates the feedback.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  61. Genoux, D. et al. Protein phosphatase 1 is a molecular constraint on learning and memory. Nature 418, 970–974 (2002).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Farah, C. A., Weatherill, D., Dunn, T. W. & Sossin, W. S. PKC differentially translocates during spaced and massed training in Aplysia. J. Neurosci. 29, 10281–10286 (2009).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  63. Tully, T., Preat, T., Boynton, S. & Del Vecchio, M. Genetic dissection of consolidated memory in Drosophila melanogaster. Cell 79, 35–47 (1994).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Yin, J. C., Del Vecchio, M., Zhou, H. & Tully, T. CREB as a memory modulator: induced expression of a dCREB2 activator isoform enhances long-term memory in Drosophila. Cell 81, 107–115 (1995).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Smolen, P., Baxter, D. A. & Byrne, J. H. Frequency selectivity, multistability, and oscillations emerge from models of genetic regulatory systems. Am. J. Physiol. 274, C531–C542 (1998).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Philips, G. T., Tzvetkova, E. I. & Carew, T. J. Transient mitogen-activated protein kinase activation is confined to a narrow temporal window required for the induction of two-trial long term memory in Aplysia. J. Neurosci. 27, 13701–13705 (2007).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  67. Seese, R. R., Wang, K., Yao, Y. Q., Lynch, G. & Gall, C. M. Spaced training rescues memory and ERK1/2 signaling in fragile X syndrome model mice. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 111, 16907–16912 (2014).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Rosenblum, K. et al. The role of extracellular regulated kinases I/II in late-phase long-term potentiation. J. Neurosci. 22, 5432–5441 (2002).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  69. Ajay, S. M. & Bhalla, U. S. A propagating ERKII switch forms zones of elevated dendritic activation correlated with plasticity. HFSP J. 1, 49–66 (2007).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  70. Chen, C. et al. Visualizing long-term memory formation in two neurons of the Drosophila brain. Science 335, 678–685 (2012).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Cepeda, N. J. et al. Optimizing distributed practice: theoretical analysis and practical implications. Exp. Psychol. 56, 236–246 (2009).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Wei, Y. & Koulakov, A. A. Long-term memory stabilized by noise-induced rehearsal. J. Neurosci. 34, 15804–15815 (2014).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  73. Cui, Z. et al. Inducible and reversible NR1 knockout reveals a crucial role of the NMDA receptor in preserving remote memories in the brain. Neuron 41, 781–793 (2004). This study demonstrates the key point that ongoing neural activity and NMDA receptor activation are essential for long-lasting preservation of stored memory.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Tetzlaff, C., Kolodziejski, C., Timme, M., Tsodyks, M. & Worgotter, F. Synaptic scaling enables dynamically distinct short- and long-term memory formation. PLoS Comput. Biol. 9, e1003307 (2013).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  75. Miller, P., Zhabotinsky, A. M., Lisman, J. E. & Wang, X. J. The stability of a stochastic CaMKII switch: dependence on the number of enzyme molecules and protein turnover. PLoS Biol. 3, e107 (2005).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  76. Pettigrew, D. B., Smolen, P., Baxter, D. A. & Byrne, J. H. Dynamic properties of regulatory motifs associated with induction of three temporal domains of memory in Aplysia. J. Comput. Neurosci. 18, 163–181 (2005).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Shema, R., Sacktor, T. C. & Dudai, Y. Rapid erasure of long-term memory associations in the cortex by an inhibitor of PKMζ. Science 317, 951–953 (2007).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Volk, L. J., Bachman, J. L., Johnson, R., Yu, Y. & Huganir, R. L. PKM-ζ is not required for hippocampal synaptic plasticity, learning and memory. Nature 493, 420–423 (2013).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  79. Fioriti, L. et al. The persistence of hippocampal-based memory requires protein synthesis mediated by the prion-like protein CPEB3. Neuron 86, 1433–1448 (2015).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Ajay, S. M. & Bhalla, U. S. A role for ERKII in synaptic pattern selectivity on the time scale of minutes. Eur. J. Neurosci. 20, 2671–2680 (2004).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Frey, U. & Morris, R. G. Synaptic tagging: implications for late maintenance of hippocampal long-term potentiation. Trends Neurosci. 21, 181–188 (1998).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  82. Babayan, A. H. et al. Integrin dynamics produce a delayed stage of long-term potentiation and memory consolidation. J. Neurosci. 32, 12854–12861 (2012).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  83. Callan, D. E. & Schweighofer, N. Neural correlates of the spacing effect in explicit verbal semantic encoding support the deficient-processing theory. Hum. Brain Mapp. 31, 645–659 (2010).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. Karpicke, J. D. & Roediger, H. L. Is expanding retrieval a superior method for learning text materials? Mem. Cognit. 38, 116–124 (2010).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. Zhang, Y. et al. Computational design of enhanced learning protocols. Nat. Neurosci. 15, 294–297 (2012). This study found that computational models describing long-term synaptic strengthening can successfully predict optimal spaced training protocols, with irregular inter-trial intervals, that enhance learning.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  86. Bhalla, U. S. & Iyengar, R. Emergent properties of networks of biological signaling pathways. Science 283, 381–387 (1999).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  87. Kim, M., Huang, T., Abel, T. & Blackwell, K. T. Temporal sensitivity of protein kinase A activation in late-phase long term potentiation. PLoS Comput. Biol. 6, e1000691 (2010).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  88. Smolen, P., Baxter, D. A. & Byrne, J. H. A model of the roles of essential kinases in the induction and expression of late long-term potentiation. Biophys. J. 90, 2760–2775 (2006).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  89. Aslam, N. & Shouval, H. Z. Regulation of cytoplasmic polyadenylation can generate a bistable switch. BMC Syst. Biol. 6, 12–20 (2012).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  90. Liu, R. Y. et al. Deficit in long-term synaptic plasticity is rescued by a computationally predicted stimulus protocol. J. Neurosci. 33, 6944–6949 (2013).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  91. Montarolo, P. G. et al. A critical period for macromolecular synthesis in long-term heterosynaptic facilitation in Aplysia. Science 234, 1249–1254 (1986).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  92. Müller, U. & Carew, T. J. Serotonin induces temporally and mechanistically distinct phases of persistent PKA activity in Aplysia sensory neurons. Neuron 21, 1423–1434 (1998).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  93. Korzus, E., Rosenfeld, M. G. & Mayford, M. CBP histone acetyltransferase activity is a critical component of memory consolidation. Neuron 42, 961–972 (2004).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  94. Park, E. et al. Epigenetic mechanisms of Rubinstein–Taybi syndrome. Neuromolecular Med. 16, 16–24 (2014).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  95. Alarcon, J. M. et al. Chromatin acetylation, memory, and LTP are impaired in CBP+/− mice: a model for the cognitive deficit in Rubinstein–Taybi syndrome and its amelioration. Neuron 42, 947–959 (2004).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  96. Alberini, C. M. Transcription factors in long-term memory and synaptic plasticity. Physiol. Rev. 89, 121–145 (2009).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  97. Zhou, L. et al. Rescue of impaired long-term facilitation at sensorimotor synapses of Aplysia following siRNA knockdown of CREB1. J. Neurosci. 35, 1617–1626 (2015).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  98. Abel, T. & Nguyen, P. V. Regulation of hippocampus-dependent memory by cyclic AMP-dependent protein kinase. Prog. Brain Res. 169, 97–115 (2008).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  99. Bartsch, D., Casadio, A., Karl, K. A., Serodio, P. & Kandel, E. R. CREB1 encodes a nuclear activator, a repressor, and a cytoplasmic modulator that form a regulatory unit critical for long-term facilitation. Cell 95, 211–223 (1998).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  100. Kida, S. A functional role for CREB as a positive regulator of memory formation and LTP. Exp. Neurobiol. 21, 136–140 (2012).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  101. Bartsch, D. et al. Aplysia CREB2 represses long-term facilitation: relief of repression converts transient facilitation into long-term functional and structural change. Cell 83, 979–992 (1995).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  102. Dong, C., Upadhya, S. C., Ding, L., Smith, T. K. & Hegde, A. N. Proteosome inhibition enhances the induction and impairs the maintenance of late-phase long-term potentiation. Learn. Mem. 15, 335–347 (2008).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  103. Pasini, S., Corona, C., Liu, J., Greene, L. A. & Shelanski, M. L. Specific downregulation of hippocampal ATF4 reveals a necessary role in synaptic plasticity and memory. Cell Rep. 11, 183–191 (2015).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  104. Smolen, P., Baxter, D. A. & Byrne, J. H. Simulations suggest pharmacological methods for rescuing long-term potentiation. J. Theor. Biol. 360, 243–250 (2014).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  105. Cuadrado-Tejedor, M. et al. Concomitant histone deacetylase and phosphodiesterase 5 inhibition synergistically prevents the disruption in synaptic plasticity and it reverses cognitive impairment in a mouse model of Alzheimer's disease. Clin. Epigenetics 7, 108 (2015).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  106. Ikegaya, Y. et al. Synfire chains and cortical songs: temporal modules of cortical activity. Science 304, 559–564 (2004).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  107. Miller, J. E., Ayzenshtat, I., Carrillo-Reid, L. & Yuste, R. Visual stimuli recruit intrinsically generated cortical ensembles. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 111, E4053–E4061 (2014).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  108. Karlsson, M. P. & Frank, L. M. Awake replay of remote experiences in the hippocampus. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 913–919 (2009).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  109. Wu, X. & Foster, D. J. Hippocampal replay captures the unique topological structure of a novel environment. J. Neurosci. 34, 6459–6469 (2014).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  110. Hsiang, H. L. et al. Manipulating a “cocaine engram” in mice. J. Neurosci. 34, 14115–14127 (2014).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  111. Lynch, G., Cox, C. D. & Gall, C. M. Pharmacological enhancement of memory or cognition in normal subjects. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 8, 90 (2014).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by US National Institutes of Health grants NS073974 and NS019895.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to John H. Byrne.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

PowerPoint slides

Glossary

Memory extinction

The decline of a learned behavioural response to a conditioned stimulus following the withdrawal of reinforcement stimuli that were previously paired with repetitions of the conditioned stimulus.

Reinforcement

A broad term used here to describe a stimulus or item that enhances the strength or lifetime of a memory.

Habituation

A decrease in the behavioural response to a stimulus following frequent repetitions of that stimulus; this term is distinct from extinction, because habituation can denote a decrease in response to a stimulus that was never paired with a reinforcing stimulus.

Memory reactivations

These are reinstatements of conditioned behavioural responses or of neural activity associated with specific responses and can be elicited by presentation of a conditioning stimulus or of the context in which learning previously occurred, or be spontaneous, occurring as a part of normal ongoing neural activity.

Drug synergism

In combined-drug treatment, a synergistic effect of the combination is an effect that is greater than that which would be predicted by considering the individual drugs as independent and not interacting.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Smolen, P., Zhang, Y. & Byrne, J. The right time to learn: mechanisms and optimization of spaced learning. Nat Rev Neurosci 17, 77–88 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2015.18

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2015.18

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing